From the Ordinary to the Extraordinary
A review of Learning to Imagine: The Science of Discovering New Possibilities by Andrew Shtulman. Reviewed by Anondah Saide and Amanda Neuwirth
Note from Michael Shermer: I have known Andrew Shtulman for many years and find his research on the cognitive psychology of scientific thinking to be at once insightful and instructive. He has been on my podcast twice, the first time for his book Scienceblind: Why Our Intuitive Theories About the World Are So Often Wrong, and again for his latest book, Learning to Imagine: The Science of Discovering New Possibilities. Imagination is commonly thought to be the special province of youth―the natural companion of free play and the unrestrained vistas of childhood. Then come the deadening routines and stifling regimentation of the adult world, dulling our imaginative powers. In fact, Shtulman argues, the opposite is true. Imagination is not something we inherit at birth, nor does it diminish with age. Instead, imagination grows as we do, through education and reflection. In this essay Anondah Saide and Amanda Neuwirth review the scientific research and literature on imagination and creativity, one of the most elusive of human abilities that is only now coming into focus by the lens of science.
Anondah Saide is an assistant professor at the University of North Texas in the Department of Educational Psychology. She has a Ph.D. in developmental psychology and leads the Worldview Foundations Research Team.
Amanda Neuwirth is a high school art teacher and Ph.D. student in educational psychology at the University of North Texas. Her research interests include the developmental impacts of childhood trauma and investigating the disproportionate impacts of exclusionary discipline in K-12 schools.
Rising temperatures, biodiversity loss, drought, mass migration, the spread of misinformation, inflation, infertility—these are just some of the major challenges facing societies around the globe. Generating innovative solutions to such challenges require expanding our understanding of what’s currently possible. But how do we cultivate the necessary imagination? By debunking counterproductive myths about imagination, Occidental College cognitive developmental scientist Andrew Shtulman might just provide us with a proper starting point. “Unstructured imagination succumbs to expectation,” he writes, “but imagination structured by knowledge and reflection allows for innovation.”
Imagination Myths
In Learning to Imagine: The Science of Discovering New Possibilities, Shtulman debunks the highly intuitive, yet obstructive myth that great imagination stems from a place of ignorance. Through the sharing of everyday examples and detailed experimental studies, Shtulman effectively tackles the pervasive deficit view of imagination—that it’s something we engage in a great deal during childhood and sadly lose as we get older.
On the contrary, Shtulman demonstrates that children’s imagination, relative to adults’, is constrained by what they think is physically plausible, statistically probable, and socially and morally acceptable. While some early philosophers and social scientists considered children to be stupid and their minds to be a blank slate, a contemporary pendulum swing has led to a confusing romanticization of children as “little scientists” capable of unacknowledged insight. A more rational and informed view recognizes that though children’s minds are not blank slates, they do often conflate what they’ve personally experienced with what “could be.” Children’s inability to imagine a possibility is thought of as evidence of its impossibility.
Shtulman notes how young children often deny the existence of uncommon but entirely possible events, such as finding an alligator under a bed, catching a fly with chopsticks, and a man growing a beard down to his toes. Children find these situations as impossible as eating lighting for dinner. Yes, children often engage in pretend play, but it often mimics mundane aspects of real life, such as cooking or construction. Children also often believe in magic and fantastical beings such as Santa Claus, but such myths were not spontaneously created by children, they were first endorsed by adults they trust. Children rarely generate novel solutions to problems as they tend to fixate on the rules and norms familiar to them, often correcting others when they have deviated from what is expected and sometimes become offended by “rule” violations.
Most importantly, Learning to Imagine is not only about children’s cognition; it is fundamentally a book about human reasoning and contains insights that are applicable to all of us. Shtulman sheds light on the many important ways in which adults continue to constrain their own imagination through self-interest, habit, fear, and a fixation on conforming to one’s social group. For example, we may constrain ourselves with resistance to adopting new technologies like artificial intelligence because they reduce the need for our skillset or simply engender fear of the unknown. We may resist something that requires us to change our habits (e.g., carry reusable bags), to something that forces us to take risks (e.g., trust a quickly developed vaccine), or to deviate from our in-group (e.g., advocate for a new theory or openly share an unpopular opinion).
What is Imagination, Anyway?
So, what is “imagination” anyway? Shtulman argues that it’s the ability to abstract from the here and the now to contemplate what could be and what could have been. Imagination is an evolved cognitive skill that is used for the purpose of everyday planning, predicting, and problem-solving. We imagine what we would buy at the store, how a meeting at work might go, how if we had only said something a different way, then we could have avoided that fight with our spouse, and so on. Simply put, imagination is about considering the “what’s possible?” question. Imagination can be engaged in for our own personal-subjective experiences (e.g., imagining how life would have been different if path B was chosen instead of path A), and for what might be more objectively relevant to others (e.g., works of art, new policies, or technologies). Shtulman’s book offers a discussion on expanding imagination that is objectively relevant to others—the kind that leads to collective rather than personally relevant innovation.
Even though Shtulman’s case for imagination is grounded in this “what’s possible” definition, how it is intertwined with closely related constructs such as “creativity” and “innovation” is somewhat less clear. What can be discerned from his book is that while imagination can be collaborative in the sense that we draw on human knowledge to ask, “what if,” it is largely a personal endeavor. Creativity, on the other hand, is the product of imagination that can be shared with others. Building upon imagination and creativity, innovation is the product of extraordinary imagination and can be developed and refined.
Mechanisms for Expanding What’s Possible
What are the proposed means by which we expand our knowledge, thereby improving the likelihood that we shift our imagination from the ordinary to the extraordinary? The nine middle chapters of Shtulman’s book outline three processes: learning through (1) examples, (2) principles, and (3) models.
The first mechanism, examples, involves learning about new possibilities via other people’s testimony, demonstrations, empirical discoveries, and technological creations. Through education, others’ knowledge becomes our knowledge. However, expanding our imagination through examples is the easiest but also the most limited means by which to expand our own. On one hand, new possibilities are added to our database of what could be, but in doing so, we are potentially limited by overly fixating on the suboptimal (yet adequate) solution we have learned; as Shtulman notes, we “privilege [our] expectation over observation.” For example, we tend to copy the necessary and unnecessary actions of others when trying to achieve the same goal—we fixate on the solution we are familiar with rather than engage in the little effort required to abstract a more efficient solution. Children are even more susceptible to such a process. For example, imagine you see a toy with a handle that is stuck at the bottom of a long tube, and you are provided with a straight pipe cleaner. How might you reach and retrieve the toy? You likely imagine bending the pipe cleaner, yet most preschoolers tasked to reach the toy in this scenario are unable to imagine how the pipe cleaner can be used as a sufficient tool.
The second mechanism, principles, refers to generating a new collection of possibilities by learning about abstract schema; in other words, theories about “how” and “why” things operate. These include learning about scientific/cause-and-effect, mathematical, and ethical principles. As a means for expanding imagination, principles are more valuable than examples because they can help us extrapolate possibilities from one situation and apply it across different domains. In one illustrative example, Shtulman discusses the physicist Ernest Rutherford, who won a Nobel prize in chemistry. Rutherford hypothesized (correctly) that electrons, like planets orbiting a sun, may orbit a nucleus. By using the principle of gravity and applying it in a different context, Rutherford generalized an insight from physics to innovate in the field of chemistry. Engaging with principles allows us to practice applying our knowledge and better understand novel relationships. While most of us are not scientists striving to win a Nobel prize, we can still learn new principles that expand our imagination. However, principles can be overgeneralized, and Shtulman argues that new applications should still be tested and replicated to confirm the connection.
The third mechanism, models, might be the most exciting as it concerns expanding our ideas about what is possible by immersing ourselves in simulated versions of reality that can be manipulated with little to no consequences. These simulations allow for personal reflection through the process of mental time travel. This includes expanding our imagination through pretense (i.e., pretend play), fiction, and religion. Pretense allows us to expand our symbolic imagination by toying with alternative possibilities somewhat rooted in reality because the real-world elements of pretend play help to make it meaningful. For example, when children and many adults are asked to draw an animal that doesn’t exist, the product is usually an amalgamation of existing animal parts rather than a completely unique creature. Such mental play supports the development of logical reasoning. Through different mediums such as books and film, fiction expands our imagination by allowing us to experience the social world through the eyes and thoughts of others. We see how others react to situations we haven’t experienced and contemplate how we might respond if we were in their shoes.
Religion is rooted less in the here and now but may enable us to expand metaphysical ideas and explore moral reasoning by directing thoughts and behavior according to the core values of a specific religion. Ultimately, models allow us to experience the lessons of working out various problems without the risks associated with acting on them in real life. On the other hand, sometimes models may communicate false information that we mark as true. Though models may sometimes lead us astray, Shtulman argues that they “provide the raw materials.... You have to represent reality before you can tinker with it, to know the facts before you can entertain counterfactuals” (p. 12).
The numerous examples that Shtulman provides for how examples, principles, and models expand imagination generate a convincing case for the central thesis of his book—that, unlike the current conventional wisdom, children’s lack of knowledge, experience, and reflection make them less imaginative than adults. However, attempts to distinguish many overlapping concepts within in the book (e.g., religious models vs. fictional models; social imagination vs. moral imagination) is sometimes disorientating. But this is not a self-help book. Instead, you’ll spend hours on an engaging (and, dare we say, nourishing) tour of the limitations and achievements of human imagination. By the end, you’ll know a lot more about how the human mind develops and reasons, and about the cognitive mechanisms that impede and enhance innovation across eras, societies, and an individual lifetime. Through your newfound knowledge, you may begin to imagine solutions you hadn’t considered before to both personal and global challenges.
This essay was a great intro to what appears to be a very interesting book. I may read it after I finish the ~10 books already piled up on my desk.
For the last ~5-10 years I have been "toying" (imagining???) with the idea that many of the ills with which we humans are burdened stem from what I've been referring to as "misuse of imagination". It's pretty obvious to me that most, if not all, religions are the end result of the "misuse of imagination". Ditto the patterns of thought which result in what we are familiar with as "conspiracy theories", as well as UFO/UAP/ETI/"alien" encounters, etc. These blatant deviations from reality, much like what goes on a child's mind with regard to Santa Claus, the Easter Bunny, etc., are the result of the human imagination being misused, or being "short-circuited" by some form of chemical or physical process which breaks apart rational thought processes. This may even help to explain what is going on in the minds of people who are afflicted with what is commonly referred to as "mental illness". Perhaps their imagination has gone hay-wire, and what was once a usable tool, brought about by its success throughout our evolutionary past, has now been turned against that individual with such things as hearing voices; uncontrollable emotional outbursts; etc.
Not far removed from the misuse of imagination which is evidenced by religious belief, is a far more serious mental deficiency which manifests itself in adherence to cults & other potentially harmful group allegiances. I'd say it's not far from reality to assert that the MAGA cohort who follow DJT are likely in the throes of a HUGE misuse of their imaginations as they mirror in their own minds the lies & deviations from reality which are implanted in their brains by the leader of this particularly dangerous social contagion. Ditto for those who misuse their imaginations to create or duplicate what's been termed the "woke mind virus" within their own minds. QR
Perhaps children are hesitant to imagine due to their attempts to conform to “ adult society norms”. Psychologically some of us are conformists, while some are more contrarian. So personality may play an effect on imagine. Actually the conformist personality may affect adult imagination as well.